ADHD | ||
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder | ||
References DSM-IV
Description & Symptoms Other
Perspectives (Intro Page) Russell Barkley's Critique of DSM-IV Neurological Model Drug Reactions (Breggin) Barkley's Model |
ADHD: definition, diagnosis, prevalence Editor's Note, December 26, 2011: I strongly urge the reader to obtain a copy of Anatomy of an Epidemic, by Robert Whitaker. It provides overwhelming evidence, citing many large studies done in the U. S. and other nations, that drug treatment for psychiatric disorders tends to worsen mental illness, and to turn brief, treatable episodes into chronic mental illness. Children who are excessively active, are unable to sustain their attention, and are deficient in their impulse control to a degree that is deviant for their developmental level are now given the clinical diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Incidence is approximately 3% - 7% of school-age children.
History of the disorder
By the 1970s, research emphasized the importance of problems with sustained attention and impulse control in addition to hyperactivity.
In the '80s, a more complex understanding of the disorder recognized
four major deficits: 1) investment, organization, and maintenance of attention and
effort; 2) ability to inhibit impulse of behavior;
Inattention: Hyperactive-Impulsive Behavior (Disinhibition) Compared to children of the same age, ADHD children are objectively more active than other children, less mature in controlling motor overflow movements, less able to stop ongoing behavior, talk more and interrupt others' conversations, less able to resist immediate temptation and delay gratification, and respond too quickly, as seen in impulsive errors on continuous performance tests. Differences in activity and impulsiveness are found between children with ADHD and those with learning disabilities and other psychiatric disorders. The symptoms are first observed around three to four years of age. Symptoms relating to hyperactivity-impulsivity typically decline in severity with advancing age. Symptoms relating to inattentiveness are more persistent, but generally appeared to decline by adolescence. This pattern may well be to some extent an artifact of the expectations of normal behavior and demands made upon children at various ages. Children under 3 yrs are normally more impulsive and hyperactive; until children enter school, few serious demands for sustained attentiveness and task completion are made.
Other characteristics: situational and contextual factors
*Greater variability than normals in performance on continuous performance tasks.
*Symptoms vary with context: ADHD children can play video games for prolonged periods of time,
but not sustain attention to schoolwork or chores.
It is not clear that ADHD-I (inattentive) is actually a subtype of ADHD, sharing a common attention deficit with the other types. It is also unclear whether ADHDHI is really a separate subtype from ADHD-C or simply an earlier developmental stage of it. The former is mostly found in preschool-age children, while the latter is seen mostly in school-age children. One would expect this given that hyperactive-impulsive symptoms appear first, followed by those of inattention. This also raises the issue of whether or not the requirement for significant inattention to diagnose ADHD is even necessary in the case of younger children. However, it must be noted that follow up studies tend to show that the hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, although often emerging before those of inattention, declined more sharply with age than do those of inattention. Such apparent contradictions need to be addressed and explained by any new theoretical model of the disorder. The present view of ADHD gives no clue to the resolution of such findings. Another problem is how well the diagnostic thresholds established for the symptom lists apply to age groups outside of those used in the DSM-IV field trials (ages 4-16 years, chiefly). If ADHD is a category of pathology than a listing of its symptoms should suffice to define it across the lifespan. But if ADHD is a psychological trait that is dimensional in nature and varies in degree in the normal population, and if that essence is not one of attention or some other developmental process, than the approach taken in DSM will fall short. The same lists and thresholds will not apply equally well across development. Abilities that are developmental in nature can vary in their manifestations at different developmental stages even though the same underlying mental ability is at work in each stage. Mental retardation is a good analogy. It is a deficiency defined as such relative to one's peer group and to an individual's stage of development on measures of intelligence and adaptive functioning. If it were defined and diagnosed like ADHD using a fixed set of items and thresholds developed exclusively on children it might be useful for identifying children as having M. R. upon entry into school-age, but would fail at diagnosing adolescents or adults having mild to moderate MR. ADHD, like MR, has come to be viewed as a developmental disorder. Therefore, the same types of adjustments to the diagnostic criteria for ADHD may need to be made as has been done for MR. When symptoms of ADHD persisting into adulthood are assessed by self-report using DSM criteria, persistence into adulthood was only three percent. When age-adjusted definitions were used, again just with self-reported symptoms, the persistence increased to 28 percent. Using the same adjusted criteria, parent reports yielded a persistence of 58 percent. This definitional problem likely has much to do with the apparent high rate of recovery from the disorder. Top Of Page |
References DSM-IV Description & Symptoms Other Perspectives (Intro Page) Russell Barkley's Critique of DSM-IV Neurological Model Drug Reactions (Breggin) Barkley's Model |