ADHD
        Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
return to ADHD home page                    return to previous page    

Treatment Alternatives for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
L. Eugene Arnold, M.D., M.Ed.

Alternate treatments (Tx) are defined for this conference as any treatment other than prescription drugs or standard behavioral treatments. In contrast with those two established general treatments, many alternate treatments are etiologically targeted (see Table 1) and consequently applicable to a smaller subpopulation of patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Therefore, scientific evaluation and clinical use of such treatments require a deeper level of diagnosis than the phenomenological criteria of DSM-IV.**

Elimination Diets (Oligoantigenic or Few-Food Diets)

The 1982 consensus development conference on defined diets in hyperactivity (NIH, 1982) called for more controlled research. Since then, at least seven controlled studies (Breakey, 1997) have demonstrated either significant improvement compared with a placebo condition (disguised full diet) (Kaplan, McNicol, Conte, et al., 1989) or deterioration on a placebo-controlled challenge of offending substances after an open diet trial and open challenge to identify the substance (Egger, Carter, Graham, et al., 1985; Pollock, Warner, 1990; Carter, Urbanowicz, Hemsley, et al., 1993; Rowe, Rowe, 1994; Boris, Mandel, 1994; Schmidt, Mocks, Lay, et al., 1997). The finding of scientifically acceptable documentation of efficacy since 1982 appears associated with broadening the range of suspected food items, selecting subjects more carefully (e.g., for allergic diathesis), and allowing for the timing peculiarities of food sensitivities. A related Tx possibility arises from the documentation of successful desensitization to the offending food by enzyme-potentiated desensitization (Egger, Stolla, McEwen, 1992). The main scientific task is to refine the diagnostic characteristics of diet responders and delineate what percentage they constitute of the ADHD population. Preliminary evidence suggests that the profile of a probable responder is a middle- or upper-class preschooler with atopy and prominent irritability and sleep disturbance, with physical as well as behavioral symptoms.

A related dietary strategy, simple elimination of sugar or candy, has not garnered convincing scientific support from repeated placebo-controlled challenge studies (Krummel, Seligson, Guthrie, 1996) despite a few encouraging reports (e.g., Goldman, Lerman, Contois, et al., 1986).    return to ADHD home page         return to previous page    

Nutritional Supplements. Both macronutrients (amino acids, lipids, carbohydrates) and micronutrients (vitamins and minerals) have been proposed as Tx for ADHD.

Table 1. Scientific status of alternate treatments for ADHD
Treatment Etiology or Mechanism Type of Data ES or p Rating* (0-6); Recommendation Risks
Few-foods diet (oligoantigenic) Food or additive sensitivity Controlled trial; placebo challenges ES 0.5-1.5 p,.05-.001 5; Define subgroup (profile; % ADHD) Nuisance, expense, nutrition
Enzyme-potentiated desensitization Food or additive sensitivity Controlled comparison with placebo injections p,.001 4; Replication Define subgroup Injection
Sugar elimination Sugar malaise Placebo-controlled challenges p>.1 0 for acute; Take FH of DM Delay std Tx
Amino acid supplementation Precursors of catecholamines Placebo-controlled comparisons ES up to 0.6, p,.01 0 despite short- lived effect of little utility Eosinophilia, neurotoxicity
Essential fatty acid supplementation Prostaglandins neur. membrane Serum level cf. cntrl plac-contr. trials ES 0.5 .1>p>.05 3; trials of n-3 Upsetting balance
Glyconutritional supplementation Need for glycoconjugates Open trials, SNAP-IV, blind teachers p,.05-.002 3; placebo trials Upsetting balance
Vitamins Deficiency vs. Idiopathic need for higher dose Placebo-controlled trials megavitamin cocktails, not RDA Megadose cocktail no benefit 0 for megacocktail; 1 for RDA, specific megavit; pilot trials Hepatotoxicity, neuropathy in megadose
Iron supplementation Co-factor make catecholamines Open trial supplementation ES 1.0 p<.05 3 † ; controlled trials Hemochroma- tosis
Zinc supplementation Co-factor for many enzymes Comparison Zn lvl of ADHD with control ES 2.4 p<.001 2 † ; controlled trials Excess
Magnesium supplementation Deficiency cf. to controls Open trial with control group ES 1.2-1.4 p<.05 3 † ; placebo trials Aggression from excess
Chinese herbals Clinical exper. Open trials, one with MPH control p<.05; no diff. MPH 3; placebo trials Delay of other Tx
Other herbals Clinical exper. No data N.A. 1; pilot trials Delay Tx
Homeopathic prep Clinical exper. No data N.A. 1; pilot trials Delay Tx
Laser acupuncture Stimulate foci for calming Open trial ES 1.0 2; controlled trial Delay other Tx, burn
EEG biofeedback Suppress theta, increase beta Open & randomized wait list ctrl trials p<0.05 3; sham-controlled trial Expense, time
EMG biofeedbck, relaxatn, hypnosis Lower arousal, muscle tone Randomized trials with controls ES 1.0-1.3 p<0.01 0 for hypnosis; 4 for EMG/relaxn; cf. med Delay other Tx
Meditation Autonomic effect focused attn Cf. relaxation, wait list ctrl, med p<.05 3; rigorous replication, sham ctrl Delay other Tx
Channel-specific perceptual training Basic readiness skills, focus Randmzd prev trial with 2 control grps ES 0.9 p<0.01 3; controlled Tx trials Delay other Tx
Vestibular stimulation Modulate behav attn, perception Open and single- blind trials ES 0.4-1.2 p ns-0.001 3; randomized sham-controlled trials Nausea, accident
Antifungal Tx GI yeast No systematic data N.A. 1; pilot trials Med risk
Thyroid Tx Thyroid Fx affects AD Sx Placebo trial: 5/8 GRTH, 1/9 other ns if thyr not abnrml 0 if thyroid nl; 6 if thyroid abnl Thyroid toxicity
Deleading Lead toxicity causes AD Sx Placebo-ctrl trial of chelation (=MPH) ES 0.7-1.6 p,.05-.001 4 if blood Pb>20; 2 if Pb<20; ctrl trial Toxicity of chelator

* Ratings: 0 = not worth considering further (despite, in the case of amino acids, some evidence of short-lived effect); 1 = credible hypothesis or collateral support or wide clinical experience, needs pilot data; 2 = promising systematic data, but not prospective trial; 3 = promising prospective data (perhaps with random assignment to control or objective/blind measures) lacking some important control -OR- controlled trial(s) with trends suggesting further exploration; 4 = one significant double-blind controlled trial needing replication -OR- multiple positive controlled trials in a treatment not easily blinded; 5 = convincing double-blind controlled evidence but needs further refinement (e.g., define target subgroup) for clinical application; 6 = should be considered established Tx for the appropriate subgroup.

† The rating would be 6 for patients showing frank deficiency of vitamins, iron, zinc, or other nutrients.

 return to ADHD home page                    return to previous page    

 

Amino Acid Supplementation. Amino acid supplementation is theoretically supported by reports of low levels of amino acids in ADHD, including the precursors of catecholamines and serotonin (Bornstein, Baker, Carroll, et al., 1990; Baker, Bornstein, Rouget, et al., 1991). Several open and controlled studies reported a short-term benefit from tryptophan, tyrosine, or phenylalanine supplementation (Nemzer, Arnold, Votolato, et al., 1986; Reimherr, Wender, Wood, et al., 1987; Wood, Reimherr, Wender, et al., 1985a). However, no lasting benefit beyond 2 to 3 months has been demonstrated (tolerance develops) (Wood, Reimherr, Wender, et al., 1985b), and even short-term benefit was not found in some studies (Eisenberg, Asnis, van Praag, et al., 1988; Zametkin, Karoum, Rapoport, 1987; Ghose, 1983). Further, such supplementation, while originally considered benign, may carry real dangers beyond that of eosinophilia. Therefore, amino acid supplementation does not appear a promising area to explore further.

Essential Fatty Acid Supplementation. Neuronal membranes are composed of phospholipids containing large amounts of polyunsaturated fatty acids, especially the n-3 and n-6 acids, which humans cannot manufacture de novo and hence are essential in the diet. Essential fatty acids (EFA) are also metabolized to prostaglandins, which modify many metabolic processes. Both the n-3 series (progenitor alpha-linolenic acid) and the n-6 series (progenitor linoleic acid) have been reported to be significantly lower in children with ADHD than in comparison controls (Mitchell, Lewis, Cutler, 1983; Mitchell, Aman, Turbott, et al., 1987; Stevens, Zentall, Deck, et al., 1995). Even total serum-free fatty acids were lower in ADHD, with ES = 2.4; p<.001 (Bekaroglu, Yakup, Yusof, et al., 1996). Aggression has been significantly inhibited in young adults by docosohexaenoic acid of the n-3 series (Hamazaki, Sawazaki, Itomura, et al., 1996). Two double-blind placebo-controlled trials of gamma-linolenic acid (n-6 series) supplementation yielded equivocal results from ADHD subjects not selected for low n-6 acids (Aman, Mitchell, Turbott, 1987; Arnold, Kleykamp, Votolato, et al., 1989); in one, the serum triglyceride gamma-linolenic acid correlated inversely with Conners scale scores (Arnold, Kleykamp, Votolato, et al., 1994). A controlled pilot trial of n-3 supplementation in ADHD subjects selected for symptoms of EFA deficiency showed a trend of advantage for the supplement despite a huge placebo effect (pre-post ES 1.8 vs. 1.4), and changes in serum phospholipid n-3 acids correlated negatively with changes in Conners scores (Burgess, Stevens, 1998). The data suggest further controlled trials in subjects selected for low serum levels.

Glyconutritional Supplements. Glyconutritional supplement contains basic saccharides necessary for cell communication and formation of glycoproteins and glycolipids: glucose, galactose, mannose, N-acetylneuraminic acid, fucose, N-acetylgalactosamine, and xylose. Only the first two are abundant in the ordinary diet. Dykman and Dykman (1998) found in an open trial of glyconutritional and phytonutritional (flash freeze-dried fruits and vegetables) supplements with 17 ADHD subjects a significant (p<.01) reduction in parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings. Dykman and McKinley (1997) found in a second open trial with the same supplements in 18 children reductions in parent inattention ratings from 2.47 to 2.05 (p<.05) and hyperactivity-impulsivity ratings from 2.23 to 1.54 (p<.002), sustained for 6 weeks. Placebo-controlled trials are needed.

Vitamin Supplementation. Three strategies for vitamin supplementation are (1) RDA multivitamin preparations, (2) megavitamin cocktails, and (3) megadoses of specific vitamins. The first is noncontroversial, but no research has been done on its effects in diagnosed ADHD, even though some reports suggest mild deficiencies in diet and blood levels that might be addressed. However, in a randomly assigned double-blind placebo-controlled trial of RDA vitamin and mineral supplementation in 47 6-year-old children not selected for ADHD, Benton and Cook (1991) found an 8.3 point IQ advantage (p<.001), mainly in nonverbal ability, an increase in concentration and decreased fidgeting on a frustrating task (p<.05), and advantage on a reaction time task assessing sustained attention (ES = 1.3; p<.05). The second strategy has been found ineffective in double-blind placebo-controlled short (2 weeks) and longer (up to 6 months) trials in ADHD and the related comorbidity of learning disorder (Arnold, 1978; Haslam, Dalby, Rademaker, 1984; Kershner, Hawke, 1979). Further, megadosage carries risks, including hepatotoxicity (Haslam, Dalby, Rademaker, 1984; Shaywitz, Siegel, Pearson, 1977). Therefore, megavitamin cocktails are not worth pursuing. The third possibility, judicious use of single vitamins in megadosage to alter neural metabolism in specific ways, is actually more like psychopharmacology and has not been adequately explored despite some encouraging early reports (e.g., Coleman, Steinberg, Tippett, et al., 1979; Brenner, 1982).

Mineral Supplements. The main mineral candidates for supplementation are iron, zinc, magnesium, and calcium, all of which have been reported deficient in ADHD compared with matched controls (e.g., Kozielec, Starobrat-Hermelin, Kotkowiak, 1994).

1. Iron Supplementation. Iron is a co-enzyme in anabolism of catecholamines. In an open 30-day supplementation trial with 17 nonanemic boys ages 7 to 11 with ADHD, Sever and colleagues (1997) found improvement in Conners parents’ scores from 17.6 to 12.7 (ES = 1.0), but not in teacher ratings. In a double-blind placebo-controlled trial in 73 teenage nonanemic but iron-deficient girls, Bruner and colleagues (1996) found improvements in verbal learning and memory. In a trial of gastroprotected ferritin in 33 iron-deficient children, Burattini and colleagues (1990) reported a decrease of hyperactivity. Iron supplementation merits further study, with focus on whether any benefit found is confined to those with laboratory evidence of iron deficiency and with due concern for possibly toxicity of excess iron.

2. Zinc Supplementation. Animal data suggest involvement of zinc deficiency in hyperactivity (e.g., Halas, Sandstead, 1975; Sandstead, Fosmire, Halas, et al., 1977), and human deficiency syndrome includes impairment of concentration and jitters (Aggett, Harries, 1979). Zinc has been reported deficient in ADHD compared with controls, with ES up to 2.4 (p<.001) (Bekaroglu, Yakup, Yusof, et al., 1996; Toren, Sofia, Sela, et al., 1996). However, McGee and colleagues (1990) did not find a significant correlation of parent and teacher hyperactivity ratings with hair or serum zinc in the epidemiologic Dunedin sample. Arnold and colleagues (1990) reported data suggesting that stimulant response may depend on adequate zinc nutriture. Despite clinical advocacy of zinc supplementation, no systematic prospective trials could be found. The obvious need is a placebo-controlled double-blind trial of RDA zinc supplementation with pretreatment assessment of zinc status to determine whether zinc deficiency is a prerequisite for any benefit found.

3. Magnesium Supplementation. Kozielec and Starobrat-Hermelin (1997) found 95 percent of 116 children ages 9 to 12 with ADHD deficient in magnesium (34 percent by serum alone). They assigned 50 children ages 7 to 12 with DSM-IV ADHD and magnesium deficiency to 6 months open supplementation with 200 mg/day and 30 similar controls to usual treatment without magnesium; the supplemented group significantly decreased their Conners ratings compared with the control group (Starobrat-Hermelin, Kozielec, 1997). Thus, magnesium supplementation merits a placebo-controlled double-blind trial and replication by other investigators. Dosage of supplementation may be important, because animal work suggests a U-shaped behavioral dose-response curve (Izenwasser, Garcia-Valdez, Kantak, 1986).

Herbal and Homeopathic Treatments.In a randomly assigned open trial, Zhang and Huang (1990) compared a Chinese herbal cocktail (80 Ss) with methylphenidate 5-15 mg b.i.d. (20 Ss) for 1 to 3 months; 23 of 80 herbal cocktail cases were “cured” (disappearance of all clinical symptoms and no recurrence for 6 months) compared with 6 of 20 taking methylphenidate. Including improved cases, the effectiveness rates were 86 percent versus 90 percent; the groups did not differ except for lower side effects and greater IQ rise in the herbal group. In an open trial with 100 hyperkinetic children, Wang and colleagues (1995) found an effectiveness rate of 94 percent, including reduction of hyperactivity, improved attention, and improved academics from the herbal Tiaoshen Liquor. In another open trial in 66 hyperkinetic children, Sun and colleagues (1994) found an effectiveness rate of 85 percent with Yizhi wit-increasing syrup, including significant improvement in behavior, school records, and soft neurological signs. Thus the open pilot data warrant placebo-controlled double-blind trials of Chinese herbals. No systematic data in ADHD could be found for Calmplex, Defendol, Gingko biloba, hypericum, or pycnogenol, but the first few listed may be worth pilot trials based on clinical experience.

Acupuncture. Despite the popularity of acupuncture, no published systematic data in ADHD could be found. Loo (1998), in unpublished preliminary pre-post single-blind data from students in grades K to 3, found improvements in Conners 10-item scores by teachers (n = 7) from 17.0 to 12.0 and in analogous parent scores (n = 6) from 23.1 to 15.5. She noted that children with the most severe ADHD could not cooperate with the Tx.

EEG Biofeedback. Electroencephalographic (EEG) biofeedback involves induction of sensorimotor or higher beta band EEG rhythms (12-18 Hertz) and suppression of theta rhythms by visual and auditory feedback. It arose from the observation that some children with ADHD have more theta and less beta rhythm than controls and animal work demonstrating reduction of motor activity associated with sensorimotor rhythm (Shouse, Lubar, 1978; Mann, Lubar, Zimmerman, 1992). There are several promising pilot trials. Lubar (1991) and Lubar and Shouse (1977) reported that in a single-subject ABA design four hyperactive children selected for low arousal showed better behavior and work habits without stimulant at the end of all treatment (ABA) than at the beginning with or without stimulant, and their unmedicated level of undesirable behaviors dropped by over half to the level of the normal controls; three of them showed synchrony of behavior with the ABA shifts. An uncontrolled open trial with 37 hyperactive children yielded significant grade point and achievement score improvements (Lubar, 1991). In an intensive summer treatment regimen, 12 children who showed EEG changes also improved on significantly more TOVA scales than did 7 who failed to show EEG changes (Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood, et al., 1995). Linden and colleagues (1996) randomly assigned 18 children with DSM-III-R ADD/ADHD to either a wait list (n = 9) or 40 EEG biofeedback sessions over a 40-week period. The treated group showed a 9 point IQ rise compared with the wait list rise of less than 1 point (p<.05) and a 28 percent reduction in the SNAP inattention score compared with a 4 percent increase in the wait list group (p<.05). Thus, this treatment merits a sham-controlled randomized trial.

EMG Biofeedback, Relaxation Training, and Hypnosis. These three related Tx modalities are typically used in some combination. The few published data on hypnotherapy or breathing control alone for ADHD are discouraging (e.g., Calhoun, Bolton, 1986; Simpson, Nelson, 1974). However, the hypnotic techniques of imagery and progressive relaxation have often been incorporated into successful EMG biofeedback protocols. There are more literature citations for EMG than for EEG biofeedback (Lee, 1991). Denkowski and colleagues (1983) randomly assigned hyperactive junior high boys to six 25-minute EMG-assisted relaxation training sessions (n = 24) or a control condition (n = 24); the treated group attained significantly higher reading and language performance and made a significant internal shift in locus of control. In 10 hyperactive boys ages 6 to 12, Dunn and Howell (1982) found significant improvement in behavior observations, parent ratings, and psychological tests after 10 relaxation training sessions but none after 10 neutral sessions. Omizo and Michael (1982) randomly assigned hyperactive boys ages 10 to 12 to either four sessions of EMG biofeedback-induced relaxation (n = 16) or sham treatment of equal length; compared with the sham, the relaxation induced significant improvements in attention and impulsivity on the Matching Familiar Figures test (ES = 1.0 to 1.3; p<.01). Krieger (1985) found in 27 children ages 7 to 11 with DSM-III ADHD significant improvement on Conners parent and teacher scales compared with an equal-n matched wait list control group. Success is largely moderated by baseline locus of control (Denkowski, Denkowski, Omizo, 1984). Despite recent neglect, the data suggest that EMG biofeedback-facilitated relaxation training merits further study.

Meditation. Meditation, though resulting in relaxation, is different from the preceding treatments in not directly targeting relaxation but achieving it indirectly. Kratter (1983) randomly assigned 24 children ages 7 to 12 with DSM-III ADD-H to either meditation training, progressive relaxation, or wait-list control, with 4 weeks of twice-weekly sessions; both active treatments, but not wait list, reduced impulsivity and improved scores on parent behavior scales but not teacher scales; only meditation training showed significant improvement on a test assessing selective attention. Moretti-Altuna (1987) randomly assigned 23 boys ages 6 to 12 with ADD-H to meditation training, medication, or standard therapy; meditation showed significant advantage in classroom behavior but not in parent ratings or psychological tests.

Perceptual Stimulation/Training. Perceptual and sensory stimulation and training include a wide variety of modalities, some with few or no data. The literature search found no systematic data on sensorimotor integration or optometric training for ADHD despite their widespread use. Neither were studies in ADHD found for massage, which has documented efficacy in other applications. The Interactive Metronome provides perceptual-motor concentration training with biofeedback about accuracy from motion sensors as the child taps to the beat provided by the program; open trials show improvements in timing that correlate at 0.2-0.4 with teacher ratings of attention, but there are no controlled data (Synaptec, 1998). In a single-blind prevention paradigm, Arnold and colleagues (1977) randomly assigned matched triplets and quads of first-graders selected for vulnerability on a perceptual screening battery to either 6 months of channel-specific perceptual training (n = 23), the same length of regular academic tutoring (n = 23), or no-contact control (n = 40); at 1-year followup, the trained group surpassed both control groups in blind teacher Conners ratings (p<.01), WRAT reading achievement, and Wechsler IQ (p<.05), although baseline measures were not different.

Mulligan (1996) reported significant impairment of vestibular processing in 309 children with ADHD compared with 309 matched children without ADHD (p<.01). In a single-blind crossover in 18 children with DSM-II hyperkinetic reaction, Bhatara and colleagues (1981) found improvement in Conners teacher ratings from rotational vestibular stimulation compared with a sham condition (p<.05), with benefit mainly confined to the 14 children younger than age 10 and those without comorbid conduct disorder. In another single-blind crossover with 12 children identified through teacher scale screening, Arnold and colleagues (1985) found an ES of 0.5 between vestibular rotational stimulation alone and two control conditions (missing significance at the sample size), compared with an ES of 0.2 between visual rotational stimulation alone and the same control conditions in a similar group of 18 children. The Comprehensive Motion Apparatus provides vestibular stimulation in all vectors through complex motion; an open trial in 14 dyslexic children (mean age, 12 ± 2.6 years) showed pre-post improvement in parent rating of attention (ES = 1.5; p<.003) and objective cognitive/achievement tests (ES = 0.4-1.2; p = .05-.001) (Stillman, 1998). Thus, stimulation and/or training of specific perceptual channels merit further research in controlled trials, especially targeting subgroups that test deficient in the particular perceptual modality.

Antifungal Treatment. Treatment with antifungal agents such as nystatin (in combination with sugar restriction and other measures) is advocated by Crook (1985, 1989, 1991) and others on the hypothesis that repeated antibiotic use for otitis media changes intestinal flora, allowing yeast overgrowth, which compromises immune function and changes the gut mucosal barrier to allow absorption of food antigens. Several components of this hypothesis are supported by collateral documentation from other fields, and the hypothesis would make sense of the reported association of chronic high sugar intake with ADHD symptoms (e.g., Prinz, Riddle, 1986) without acute effects, in that sugar could promote yeast overgrowth chronically without showing acute effects on behavior. However, this hypothesis is not supported by any systematic prospective trial data in ADHD, and a trial of nystatin alone for another syndrome (fatigue, premenstrual tension, gastrointestinal symptoms, and depression) was negative (Dismukes, Wade, Lee, et al., 1990). A systematic randomly assigned trial in ADHD should be carried out, preferably double-blind placebo-controlled and accompanied by the sugar restriction and other supportive measures recommended by the advocates of this treatment.

Thyroid Treatment. Despite initial enthusiasm about resistance to thyroid hormone as a key to a large proportion of ADHD, this genetic syndrome appears extremely rare in ADHD samples. The same studies, however, reveal a rate of other thyroid dysfunction ranging from 2 percent to 5 percent (e.g., Weiss, Stein, Trommer, et al., 1993; Valentine, Rossi, O’Leary, et al., 1997), and the rate may be higher in those with comorbid mood disorder (West, Sax, Stanton, et al., 1996). In children with thyroid dysfunction, it seems related to attentional and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (Rovet, Alvarez, 1996; Hauser, Soler, Brucker-Davis, et al., 1997). In a double-blind placebo crossover trial of thyroid supplementation, only one of nine children with ADHD and normal thyroid function improved compared with five of eight with ADHD and resistance to thyroid hormone (Weiss, Stein, Refetoff, 1997). Thus, thyroid treatment does not seem promising in children with ADHD with normal thyroid function but would seem the treatment of choice for those with thyroid dysfunction. Therefore, all children with ADHD should be screened for historical and physical exam signs of possible thyroid dysfunction (Weiss, Stein, in press).

Deleading. Animal data (e.g., Silbergeld, Goldberg, 1975) document hyperactivity as one symptom of chronic lead poisoning and suggest that lead-induced hyperactivity depends on blood lead levels and can be reversed by chelation (Gong, Evans, 1997). In humans, the level considered toxic for subtle neuropsychiatric symptoms has declined with increasing knowledge, with some authors placing it as low as single digits (Kahn, Kelly, Walker, 1995) and many recommending 10 mcg/dL as the threshold. Whether such lead levels correlate with behavioral and cognitive measures is the subject of some controversy, partly depending on the sample size and consequent power. David and colleagues (1976) openly treated 13 children who had hyperkinetic (HK) reaction and blood lead levels greater than 25mcg/dL with penicillamine (CaEDTA if allergic to penicillin); the 7 with no other probable medical cause of their HK reaction improved in teacher hyperactivity rating (ES = 1.4; p<.01) and parent hyperactive-impulsive rating (ES = 2.2; p<.05) but not significantly in teacher inattention rating (ES = 0.6), whereas the 6 with another probable medical cause did not improve. In a double-blind placebo-controlled 12-week trial, David and colleagues (1983) randomly assigned hyperactive children with “minimally elevated lead levels” (mean, 28 ± 6 mcg/dL) to either penicillamine plus methylphenidate placebo (n = 22), methylphenidate (5-40 mg/day) plus penicillamine placebo (n = 11), or double placebo (n = 11); compared with placebo, penicillamine improved Conners teacher hyperactivity scores (ES = 1.6; p<.001), parent Werry-Weiss-Peters hyperactivity scores (ES = 0.7; p<.05), and CGI (ES = 1.4; p<.01); across measures the penicillamine group did nonsignificantly better than the methylphenidate group. Thus, it appears that deleading would be the treatment of choice for children with ADHD who have blood lead elevations in the range treated by Oliver and associates. To how low a blood lead level this treatment should extend is a research question of high priority.
return to ADHD home page                    return to previous page    

References

Agget PJ, Harries JT. Current status of zinc in health and disease states. Arch Dis Child 1979;54:909-17.

Aman MG, Mitchell EA, Turbott SH. The effects of essential fatty acid supplementation by Efamol in hyperactive children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1987;15:75-90.

Arnold LE. Megavitamins for MBD: a placebo-controlled study. JAMA 1978;20:24.

Arnold LE, Barnebey N, McManus J, Smeltzer D, Conrad A, Winer G, Desgranges L. Prevention by specific perceptual remediation for vulnerable first-graders: controlled study and follow-up of lasting effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977;34:l279-94.

Arnold LE, Clark DL, Sachs LA, Jakim S, Smithies C. Vestibular and visual rotational stimulation as treatment for attention deficit and hyperactivity. Am J Occup Ther 1985;39:2, 84-9l.

Arnold LE, Kleykamp D, Votolato NA, Gibson RA, Horrocks L. Potential link between dietary intake of fatty acids and behavior: pilot exploration of serum lipids in ADHD. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 1994;4(3):171-80.

Arnold LE, Kleykamp D, Votolato NA, Taylor WA, Kontras SB, Tobin K. Gamma-linolenic acid for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: placebo controlled comparison to d-amphetamine. Biol Psychiatry 1989;25:222-8.

Arnold LE, Votolato NA, Kleykamp D, Baker GB, Bornstein, RA. Does hair zinc predict amphetamine improvement of ADHD hyperactivity? Int J Neurosci 1990;50:103-7.

Baker GB, Bornstein RA, Rouget AC, Ashton SE, van Muyden JC, Coutts RT. Phenylethylaminergic mechanisms in attention-deficit disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1991;29:15-22.

Bekaroglu M, Yakup A, Yusof G, Orhan D, Hilal M, Erol E, et al. Relationships between serum free fatty acids and zinc and ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37:225-7.

Benton D, Cook R. Vitamin and mineral supplements improve the intelligence scores and concentration of six-year-old children. Pers Indiv Diff 1991;12:1151-8.

Bhatara V, Clark DL, Arnold LE, Gonsett R, Smeltzer DJ. Hyperkinesis treated by vestibular stimulation: an exploratory study. Biol Psychiatry 1981;l6(3):269-79.

Boris M, Mandel FS. Foods and additives are common causes of the attention deficit hyperactive disorder in children. Ann Allergy 1994;72:462-8.

Bornstein RA, Baker GB, Carroll A, King G, Wong JT, Douglass AB. Plasma amino acids in attention deficit disorder. Psychiatry Res 1990;33:301-6.

Breakey J. The role of diet and behaviour in childhood. J Paediatr Child Health 1997;33:190-4.

Brenner A. The effects of megadoses of selected B complex vitamins on children with hyperkinesis: controlled studies with long-term follow-up. J Learn Disabil 1982;15:258-64.

Bruner AB, Joffe A, Duggan AK, Casella F, Brandt, J. Randomized study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in non-anemic iron-deficient girls. Lancet 1996;347:992-6.

Burattini MG, Amendola F, Aufierio T, Spano M, Di Bitonto G, Del Vecchio GC, et al.

Evaluation of the effectiveness of gastro-protected proteoferrin in the therapy of sideropenic anemia in childhood. Minerva Pediatr 1990;42:343-7.

Burgess J, Stevens L. Personal communication. Purdue University, 1998.

Calhoun G Jr, Bolton JA. Hypnotherapy: a possible alternative for treating pupils affected with attention deficit disorder. Percept Mot Skills 1986;63:1191-5.

Carter CM, Urbanowicz M, Hemsley R, Mantilla L, Strobel S, Graham PJ, et al. Effects of a few food diet in attention deficit disorder. Arch Dis Child 1993;69:564-8.

Coleman M, Steinberg G, Tippett J, Bhagavan HN, Coursin DB, Gross M, et al. A preliminary study of the effect of pyridoxine administration in a subgroup of hyperkinetic children: a double-blind crossover comparison with methylphenidate. Biol Psychiatry 1979;14:741-51.

Crombie IK, Todman J, McNeill G, Florey CD, Menzies I, Kennedy RA. Effect of vitamin and mineral supplementation on verbal and nonverbal reasoning of schoolchildren. Lancet 1990;335:744-7.

Crook WG. A controlled trial of nystatin for the candidiasis hypersensitivity syndrome. N Engl J Med 1991;324:1592-4.

Crook WG. Ear infections, hyperactivity, and the yeast connection. International Health Foundation Healthline 1989;1:1.

Crook WG. Pediatricians, antibiotics, and office practice. Pediatrics 1985;76(1):139-40.

David OJ, Hoffman SP, Clark J, Grad G, Sverd J. The relationship of hyperactivity to moderately elevated lead levels. Arch Environ Health 1983;38:341-6.

David OJ, Hoffman SP, Sverd J, Clark J, Voeller K. Lead and hyperactivity. Behavioral response to chelation: a pilot study. Am J Psychiatry 1976;133:1155-8.

Denkowski KM, Denkowski GC. Is group progressive relaxation training as effective with hyperactive children as individual EMG Biofeedback? Biofeedback Self Regul 1984;9:353-64.

Denkowski KM, Denkowski GC, Omizo MM. Predictors of success in the EMG biofeedback training of hyperactive male children. Biofeedback Self Regul 1984;9:253-64.

Denkowski KM, Denkowski GC, Omizo MM. The effects of EMG-assisted relaxation training on the academic performance, locus of control, and self-esteem of hyperactive boys. Biofeedback Self Regul 1983;8:363-75.

Dismukes WE, Wade JS, Lee JY, Dockery BK, Hain JD. A randomized double-blind trial of nystatin therapy for the candidiasis hypersensitivity syndrome. N Engl J Med 1990;323:1717-23.

Dunn FM, Howell RJ. Relaxation training and its relationship to hyperactivity in boys. J Clin Psychol 1982;38:92-100.

Dykman KD, Dykman RA. Effect of nutritional supplements on attentional-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 1998;33(1):49-60.

Dykman, KD, McKinley R. Effect of glyconutritionals on the severity of ADHD. Proceedings of the Fisher Institute for Medical Research 1997;1(1):24-5.

Egger J, Carter CM, Graham PJ, Gumley D, Soothill JF. Controlled trial of oligoantigenic treatment in the hyperkinetic syndrome. Lancet 1985;1:540-5.

Egger J, Stolla A, McEwen LM. Controlled trial of hyposensitisation in children with food-induced hyperkinetic syndrome. Lancet 1992;339:1150-3.

Eisenberg J, Asnis GM, van Praag HM, Vela RM. Effect of tyrosine on attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. J Clin Psychiatry 1988;49:193-5.

Ghose KL. L-tryptophan in hyperactive child syndrome associated with epilepsy: a controlled study. Neuropsychobiology 1983;10:111-4.

Goldman JA, Lerman RH, Contois JH, Udall JN Jr. Behavioral effects of sucrose on preschool children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1986;14:565-77.

Gong Z, Evans HL. Effect of chelation with meso-dimercaptosuccinic acid (DMSA) before and after the appearance of lead-induced neurotoxicity in the rat. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 1997;144:205-14.

Halas ES, Sandstead HH. Some effects of prenatal zinc deficiency on behavior of the adult rat. Pediatr Res 1975;9:94-7.

Hamazaki T, Sawazaki S, Itomura M, Asaoka E, Nagao Y, Nishimura N, et al. The effect of docosahexaenoic acid on aggression in young adults. A placebo-controlled double-blind study. J Clin Invest 1996;97:1129-33.

Haslam RHA, Dalby JT, Rademaker AW. Effects of megavitamin therapy on children with attention deficit disorders. Pediatrics 1984;74:103-11.

Hauser P, Soler R, Brucker-Davis F, Weintraub BD. Thyroid hormones correlate with symptoms of hyperactivity but not inattention in ADHD. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1997;22:107-14.

Izenwasser SE, Garcia-Valdez K, Kantak KM. Stimulant-like effects of magnesium on aggression in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1986;25:1195-99.

Kahn CA, Kelly PC, Walker WO Jr. Lead screening in children with ADHD and developmental delay. Clin Pediatr 1995;34:498-501.

Kaplan BJ, McNicol J, Conte RA, Moghadam HK. Dietary replacement in preschool-aged hyperactive boys. Pediatrics 1989;83:7-17

Kershner J, Hawke W. Megavitamins and learning disorders: a controlled double-blind experiment. J Nutr 1979;159:819-26.

Kozielec T, Starobrat-Hermelin B. Assessment of magnesium levels in children with ADHD. Magnes Res 1997;10:143-8.

Kozielec T, Starobrat-Hermelin B, Kotkowiak L. Deficiency of certain trace elements in children with hyperactivity. Psychiatra Pol 1994;28:345-53.

Kratter J. The use of meditation in the treatment of attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Dissertation Abstracts International 1983;44:1965.

Krieger GDR. Reduction of hyperactivity using progressive muscle relaxation imagery and autogenic exercises with electromyographic biofeedback. Dissertation Abstracts International 1985;46-10 (Section B):3617.

Krummel DA, Seligson FH, Guthrie HA. Hyperactivity: is candy causal? Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 1996;36:31-47.

Lee SW. Biofeedback as a treatment for childhood hyperactivity: a critical review of the literature. Psychol Rep 1991;68:163-92.

Linden M, Habib T, Radojevic V. A controlled study of the effects of EEG biofeedback on cognition and behavior of children with attention deficit disorder and learning disabilities. Biofeedback Self Regul 1996;21:35-49.

Loo M. Laser acupuncture treatment of ADHD. Preliminary personal communication re NIMH grant, 1998.

Lubar JF. Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedback for attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorders. Biofeedback Self Regul 1991;16:201-25.

Lubar JF, Shouse MN. Use of biofeedback in the treatment of seizure disorders and hyperactivity. In: Lahey BB, Kazdin AE, editors. Advances in clinical child psychology. Vol. 1. New York: Plenum Press; 1997. p. 203-65.

Lubar JF, Swartwood MO, Swartwood JN, O’Donnell PH. Evaluation of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changes in TOVA scores, behavior ratings, and WISC-R performance. Biofeedback Self Regul 1995;20:83-99.

Mann CA, Lubar JF, Zimmerman AW, Miller CA, Muenchen RA. Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatr Neurol 1992; 8:30-6.

McGee R, Williams S, Anderson J, McKenzie-Parnell JH, Silva PA. Hyperactivity and serum and hair zinc levels in 11-year-old children from the general population. Biol Pscychiatry 1990;28:165-8.

Mitchell EA, Aman MG, Turbott SH, Manku M. Clinical characteristics and serum essential fatty acid levels in hyperactive children. Clin Pediatr 1987;6:406-11.

Mitchell EA, Lewis S, Cutler DR. Essential fatty acids and maladjusted behavior in children. Prostaglandins Leukot Med 1983;12:281-7.

Moretti-Altuna G. The effects of meditation versus medication in the treatment of attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Dissertation Abstracts International 1987;47:4658.

Mulligan S. An analysis of score patterns of children with attention disorders on the sensory integration and praxis tests. Am J Occup Ther 1996;50:647-54.

Nemzer ED, Arnold LE, Votolato NA, McConnell H. Amino acid supplementation as therapy for attention deficit disorder (ADD). J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 1986;25(4):509-13.

NIH Consensus Development Conference. Defined diets and childhood hyperactivity. Clin Pediatr 1982;21:627-630.

Omizo MM, Michael WB. Biofeedback-induced relaxation training and impulsivity, attention to task, and locus of control among hyperactive boys. J Learn Disabil 1982;5:414-6.

Pollock I, Warner JO. Effect of artificial food colors on childhood behavior. Arch Dis Child 1990;65:74-77.

Prinz RJ, Riddle DB. Associations between nutrition and behavior in 5-year-old children. Nutr Rev 1986;44 Suppl:151-8.

Reimherr FW, Wender PH, Wood DR, Ward M. An open trial of l-tyrosine in the treatment of attention deficit disorder, residual type. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:1071-3.

Rovet J, Alvarez M. Thyroid hormone and attention in school-age children with congenital hypothyroidism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37:579-85.

Rowe KS. Synthetic food colorings and hyperactivity: a double-blind crossover study. Aust Paediatr J 1988;24:143-7.

Rowe KS, Rowe KJ. Synthetic food coloring and behavior: a dose-response effect in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, repeated-measures study. J Pediatr 1994;125(5 Pt 1):691-8.

Sandstead HH, Fosmire GJ, Halas ES, Jacob RA, Strobel DA, Marks EO. Zinc deficiency: effects on brain and behavior of rats and rhesus monkeys. Teratology 1977;16:229-234.

Schmidt MH, Mocks P, Lay B, Eisert HG, Fojkar R, Fritz-Sigmund D, et al. Does oligoantigenic diet influence hyperactive/conduct-disordered children-a controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;6:88-95.

Sever Y, Ashkenazi A, Tyano S, Weizman A. Iron treatment in children with ADHD: a preliminary report. Neuropsychobiology 1997;35:178-80.

Shaywitz BA, Siegel NJ, Pearson HA. Megavitamins for minimal brain dysfunction: a potentially dangerous therapy. JAMA 1977;238:1749-50.

Shouse MN, Lubar JF. Physiological basis of hyperkinesis treated with methylphenidate. Pediatrics 1978;62:343-51.

Silbergeld EK, Goldberg AM. Pharmacological and neurochemical investigations of lead-induced hyperactivity. Neuropharmacology 1975;14:431-44.

Simpson DD, Nelson AL. Attention training through breathing control to modify hyperactivity. J Learn Disabil 1974;7:15-23.

Starobrat-Hermelin B, Kozielec T. The effects of magnesium physiological supplementation on hyperactivity in children with ADHD: positive response to magnesium oral loading test. Magnes Res 1997;10:149-156.

Stevens LJ, Zentall SS, Deck JL, Abate ML, Watkins BA, Lipp SR, et al. Essential fatty acid metabolism in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Clin Nutr 1995;62:761-8. Stillman, M. Personal communication about preliminary data from Ball State, 1998.

Sun Y, Wang Y, Qu X, Wang J, Fang J, Zhang L. Clinical observations and treatment of hyperkinesia in children by traditional Chinese medicine. J Tradit Chin Med 1994;14:105-9.

Synaptec LLC. Interactive Metronome. http://interactivemetronome.com. 1998.

Toren P, Sofia E, Sela BA, Wolmer L, Weitz R, Dov I, et al. Zinc deficiency in ADHD. Biol Psychiatry 1996;40:1308-10.

Valentine J, Rossi E, O’Leary P, Parry TS, Kurinczuk JJ, Sly P. Thyroid function in a population of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Paediatr Child Health 1997;33:117-20.

Wang LH, Li CS, Li GZ. Clinical and experimental studies on tiaoshen liquor for infantile hyperkinetic syndrome. Chung Kuo Chung Hsi I Chieh Ho Tsa Chih 1995;15:337-40.

Weiss RE, Stein MA. Thyroid function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In: Accardo, Whitman, Blondis, Stein, editors. ADHD in children and adults. In press.

Weiss RE, Stein MA, Refetoff S. Behavioral effects of liothyronine (L-T3) in children with ADHD in the presence and absence of resistance to thyroid hormone. Thyroid 1997;7:389-93.

Weiss RE, Stein MA, Trommer B, Refetoff S. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and thyroid function. J Pediatr 1993;123:539-45.

West SA, Sax KW, Stanton SP, Keck PE Jr, McElroy SL, Strakowski SM. Differences in thyroid function studies in acutely manic adolescents with and without ADHD. Psychopharmacol Bull 1996;32:63-6.

Wood DR, Reimherr FW, Wender PH. Treatment of attention deficit disorder with DL-phenylalanine. Psychiatry Res 1985a;16:21-6.

Wood DR, Reimherr FW, Wender PH. Amino acid precursors for the treatment of attention-deficit disorder, residual type. Psychopharmacol Bull 1985b;21:146-9.

Zametkin AJ, Karoum F, Rapoport J. Treatment of hyperactive children with D-phenylalanine. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:792-4.

Zhang H, Huang J. Preliminary study of traditional Chinese medicine treatment of minimal brain dysfunction: analysis of 100 cases. Chung Hsi I Chieh Ho Tsa Chih (Chinese J Modern Developments in Traditional Medicine) 1990;10:278-9
return to ADHD home page                    return to previous page